*Syntactic adaptation effects do not transfer across tasks*
Emily Atkinson,1 Ian Rigby,2 Naomi Shapiro,2 Brent Woo,2 & Akira Omaki2
1 University of Michigan; 2 University of Washington
Studies on syntactic adaptation have demonstrated that biases in ambiguity
resolution can be altered by manipulating the probability of the competing
structures in the input [1-3]. A recent experiment extended these findings
to filler-gap dependency processing [4]. The parser is biased to complete
filler-gap dependencies early (i.e., at the verb) [5] in part because
direct object (DO) gaps are the most frequent gap position [4]. Exposure to
input skewed toward an *a priori* unexpected gap position, i.e.,
prepositional object (PO) gaps (1:*wrote the post about __*), alters this
bias such that participants no longer actively complete the dependency at
the verb. However, these existing demonstrations come from studies that
manipulate the distribution of structures within a single task. In these
instances, adaptation may be the result of task-specific expectation
adaptation rather than a general adaptation of parsing biases. If
adaptation effects transfer across different tasks (as in phonetic
adaptation studies [6]), this would provide evidence that syntactic
adaptation is the result of changes to underlying parsing biases. While
previous studies have utilized blocked designs to provide concentrated
input [2,4], the current studies present this exposure as a separate task. Two
eye tracking during reading experiments provide evidence that syntactic
adaptation effects do not transfer from one task environment to another.
In both experiments, the exposure phase was masked as a
separate sentence recognition study: participants read 12 short stories
that each contained 4 target sentences and were tasked with identifying if
2 sentences were contained in the previous story. For one group of
participants, all of the target sentences contained DO gaps (1:*wrote __
about the article*), while the other group’s stories contained PO gap
target sentences (1:*wrote the post about __*).
*Exp1 *(N=48) Target sentences in both the story phase and test
phase consisted of a filler-gap dependency with a PO gap preceded by a
direct object NP (2). If the parser actively completes the dependency at
the verb, we expect a *filled gap effect* [5,7] on the DO region: a reading
time increase in the NP-fronting condition (2:*that*) compared to the
PP-fronting condition (2:*from which*). In the critical DO region, the
NP-fronting condition had longer regression paths (*β*=102.42,
*t*=4.90, *p*<0.001)
and more regressions (*β*=0.62, *Z*=3.02, *p*<0.01) than the PP-fronting
condition. There was no significant effect of story exposure group or an
interaction. Thus, exposure group did not affect filler-gap dependency
processing during the test phase as participants in both groups exhibited
the filled gap effect.
*Exp2* (N=48) While the story phase was identical to that from
Exp1, the target sentences during the test phase utilized a *plausibility
mismatch *manipulation [8] to examine active gap filling. Specifically, we
expect a slowdown on or after the verb in the implausible-filler conditions
(3:*which city…wrote*) compared to the plausible-filler conditions (3:*which
book*…*wrote*). No significant effects were found in the critical verb
region, but results from the spillover region indicate that the
plausibility manipulation was successful. Sentences with implausible
fillers had significantly longer regression path durations
(*β*=-38.24, *t*=17.95,
*p*<0.05) and significantly more regressions (*β*=-0.55, *Z*=0.18, *p*<0.01).
The effect of story exposure group and the interaction were both not
significant. As in Exp1, exposure group did not affect dependency
processing.
Taken together, these results suggest that syntactic adaptation is
not the result of generalized changes to parsing biases, but rather a more
specific shift in parsing biases that may be task-specific. Implications
for the theory that syntactic adaptation is a form of implicit learning [3]
will be discussed.
(1) *DO gap exposure / PO gap exposure:* The post that their friend wrote
__ about the article… / The article that their friend wrote the post about
__...
(2) *PP-fronting / NP-fronting:* The suitcase *from which* / *that* the
stealthy, wanted thief |V stole |DO the precious jewels | __ / *from* __
was full of sentimental items.
(3) *Plausibility Match / Mismatch:* *The book* / *The city* that |NP the
author |V wrote |Spillover thoughtfully | about __ was named for an
explorer.
*References* [1] Jaeger & Snider 2013. *Cognition*. [2] Fine et al. 2013. *PLOS
One*. [3] Fine & Jaeger 2013. *CogSci*. [4] Atkinson & Omaki 2016. CUNY
poster. [5] Stowe 1986. *LCP*. [6] Norris, McQueen, & Cutler 2003. *Cognitive
Psychology*. [7] Wagers & Phillips, 2014. *JEP*. [8] Traxler & Pickering
1996. *JML*.