*Do first language neural processes for morphosyntax transfer to the second
language? *
*Using event-related potentials to expand the evidence of cross-linguistic
influence*
Irene Finestrat, David Abugaber, Alicia Luque & Kara Morgan-Short (University
of Illinois at Chicago)
Ifines2@uic.edu
This study examines whether individual differences in first language (L1)
processing of morphosyntax transfer to second language (L2) using
event-related potentials (ERPs) as a measure of language processing. Transfer
of L1 skills to the L2 is well attested behaviorally (Sparks, 2012).
However, whether L1 processing transfers to an L2 is relatively unknown.
The present study brings together two areas of research to examine this
question. First, the field of second language acquisition aims to account
for variability in L2 performance and processing. Individual differences in
ERP responses elicited during L2 grammatical processing have been explained
by a variety of factors (Morgan-Short, 2014). For example, whether L2
learners elicit a N400 or P600 dominant response to L2 grammar has been
accounted for by motivation and age of arrival to the L2 context (e.g., Tanner,
Inoue, & Osterhout, 2014). Second, recent research in L1 processing has
evidenced quantitative and qualitative differences among native speakers (e.g.,
Pakulak & Neville, 2010). For example, L1 speakers have also been found to
vary in whether they show a P600 or N400 dominant response to L1
grammatical processing (e.g., Tanner & van Hell, 2014). Given the evidence
for individual differences in both L1 and L2 processing, we explore whether
individuals transfer their dominant processing strategy for L1
morphosyntax, i.e., either a P600 or N400, to L2 morphosyntax at early
stages of learning.
Participants were 42 native speakers of English who were L2 learners of
Spanish enrolled in third and fourth semester college classes. ERP data
were collected while they judged the grammaticality of 122 experimental
sentences, along with 122 of filler sentences, in English and Spanish in
two counterbalanced sessions. Experimental sentences were designed to
assess processing of morphosyntactic agreement (see Table 1). First, for a
subject-verb agreement condition, 60 sentences consisted of third-person
singular and plural correct and violation sentences. Similarly, for a
determiner-noun agreement condition, 62 correct and violation sentences
contained singular and plural noun phrases. The English stimuli were
translations of the Spanish sentences, but no participant was exposed to
the same sentence in both languages as the stimuli were divided across
lists. Participants read the sentences, one word at a time, and were asked
to judge the grammaticality of the sentences using a response box. EEG data
was processed following standard lab procedures. Then mean amplitude
averages were calculated for each individual in standard N400 and P600 time
windows, 300-500ms and 600-900ms, respectively.
At the group level, repeated ANOVAS were performed to examine grand average
ERP responses. In the L1, results revealed a P600 effect to both agreement
conditions. However, no language-related ERP effects were evidenced for L2
at the group level. At the individual level, each individual’s N400 and
P600 effect magnitudes were calculated in a central-parietal region of
interest (electrodes C4, Cz, C3, P4, Pz, P3) in the time windows indicated
above (see Figure 1 caption). Results revealed variability in the
distribution of brain response dominance for both L1 and L2 morphosyntactic
processing (see Figure 1) with a continuum of responses ranging from
negative-dominant (larger N400) to positive-dominant (larger P600). In
order to address the research question, we calculated each participant’s
response dominance index (RDI, Tanner, Inoue, & Osterhout, 2014; Tanner &
van Hell, 2014; Tanner, 2019), a measure of their relative response
dominance (either N400 or P600 dominant). Correlation analyses between
English and Spanish RDIs revealed that participants’ dominant ERP responses
to grammatical processing were significantly correlated across languages,
both for the SV (*r* = .75, *p *= .00) and the NP condition (*r* = .36, *p *=
.02, see Figure 2). Results suggest that ERP response dominance is a stable
trait, transferrable across languages, thereby constituting a source of
individual differences in morphosyntactic processing, regardless of the
language. Based on this pattern of results, we conclude that
cross-linguistic influence also occurs at the processing level.
*Table 1.* Grammaticality Judgment Task stimuli
Condition
Correct Control
Violation
SV agreement
La mujer *dibuja* en su habitación.
The lady *draws* in her bedroom.
*La mujer *dibujan* en su habitación.
*The lady *draw* in her bedroom.
NP agreement
La profesora enseña un *curso* este semestre.
The professor teaches a *course* this semester.
*La profesora enseña unos *curso* este semestre.
*The professor teaches a few *course* this semester.
*Note*. Italics indicate the critical word in each sentence. Violation
sentences are marked with an asterisk
*English (L1) *
*Spanish (L2)*
*Figure 1.* Distribution of N400 (grammatical minus ungrammatical) and P600
(ungrammatical minus grammatical) effect magnitudes (following Tanner & van
Hell, 2014) across participants in English and Spanish over both
conditions. Each dot represents one participant. The line represents equal
N400 and P600 effect magnitudes: Individuals to the left of the line
primarily showed an N400 effect, while individuals to the right primarily
showed a P600 effect.
*Subject Verb Agreement*
*Noun Phrase agreement*
Spanish RDI
English RDI
Spanish RDI
English RDI
*Figure 2.* Correlation between RDIs in English and Spanish for the SV
(left) and NP (right) conditions.
*REFERENCES*
Morgan-Short, K. (2014). Electrophysiological approaches to understanding
second language acquisition: A field reaching its potential.* Annual Review
of Applied Linguistics, 34*, 15-36.
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. J. (2010). Proficiency differences in syntactic
processing of monolingual native speakers indexed by event-related
potentials.* Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22*(12), 2728-2744.
doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21393
Sparks, R. L. (2012a). Individual differences in L2 learning and long‐term
L1–L2 relationships.* Language Learning, 62*(2), 5-27.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00704.x
Tanner, D. (2019). Robust neurocognitive individual differences in
grammatical agreement processing: A latent variable approach.* Cortex, 111*,
210-237. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.011
Tanner, D., & van Hell, J. G. (2014). ERPs reveal individual differences in
morphosyntactic processing.* Neuropsychologia, 56*, 289-301.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.002
Tanner, D., Inoue, K., & Osterhout, L. (2014). Brain-based individual
differences in online L2 grammatical comprehension. *Bilingualism: Language
and Cognition., 17*, 277-293. doi:10.1017/S1366728913000370